Change search
Link to record
Permanent link

Direct link
Publications (10 of 30) Show all publications
Jylhä, K. M., Rydgren, J. & Strimling, P. (2022). Xenophobia among radical and mainstream right-wing party voters: prevalence, correlates and influence on party support. Ethnic and Racial Studies, 45(16), 261-286
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Xenophobia among radical and mainstream right-wing party voters: prevalence, correlates and influence on party support
2022 (English)In: Ethnic and Racial Studies, ISSN 0141-9870, E-ISSN 1466-4356, Vol. 45, no 16, p. 261-286Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Considering the current political relevance of anti-immigration sentiments, we examined preference to avoid interacting with immigrants - conceptualized here as a manifestation of xenophobia - among radical (Sweden Democrats, Sverigedemokraterna, N = 2216) and mainstream (Conservative Party, Moderaterna, N = 634) right-wing voters in Sweden. Correlates of xenophobia did not differ between the voter groups or compared to other populations in previous research, suggesting that increased societal focus on immigration has not altered the correlation patterns. Intended Sweden Democrat (vs. Conservative Party) voting correlated with Right-Wing Authoritarianism, institutional distrust, less right-leaning socioeconomic attitudes (in both low- and high-xenophobia subgroups), sexist attitudes (low-xenophobia subgroup), male gender and younger age (high-xenophobia subgroup). In both voter groups, respondents with higher xenophobia expressed on average more sympathy for the Sweden Democrats, perhaps indicating a larger potential voter base. We discuss the interplay of xenophobia and contemporary voting behaviours, and the concept of xenophobia in relation to anti-immigration attitudes.

Keywords
Xenophobia, anti-immigrant attitudes, the radical right, Sweden Democrats, the Conservative Party, voting behaviour
National Category
Sociology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-204841 (URN)10.1080/01419870.2022.2061866 (DOI)000789363700001 ()2-s2.0-85129162274 (Scopus ID)
Available from: 2022-05-20 Created: 2022-05-20 Last updated: 2022-10-27Bibliographically approved
Jylhä, K. M., Strimling, P. & Rydgren, J. (2020). Climate Change Denial among Radical Right-Wing Supporters. Sustainability, 12(23), Article ID 10226.
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Climate Change Denial among Radical Right-Wing Supporters
2020 (English)In: Sustainability, E-ISSN 2071-1050, Vol. 12, no 23, article id 10226Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

The linkage between political right-wing orientation and climate change denial is extensively studied. However, previous research has almost exclusively focused on the mainstream right, which differs from the far right (radical and extreme) in some important domains. Thus, we investigated correlates of climate change denial among supporters of a radical right-wing party (Sweden Democrats, N = 2216), a mainstream right-wing party (the Conservative Party, Moderaterna, N = 634), and a mainstream center-left party (Social Democrats, N = 548) in Sweden. Across the analyses, distrust of public service media (Swedish Television, SVT), socioeconomic right-wing attitudes, and antifeminist attitudes outperformed the effects of anti-immigration attitudes and political distrust in explaining climate change denial, perhaps because of a lesser distinguishing capability of the latter mentioned variables. For example, virtually all Sweden Democrat supporters oppose immigration. Furthermore, the effects of party support, conservative ideologies, and belief in conspiracies were relatively weak, and vanished or substantially weakened in the full models. Our results suggest that socioeconomic attitudes (characteristic for the mainstream right) and exclusionary sociocultural attitudes and institutional distrust (characteristic for the contemporary European radical right) are important predictors of climate change denial, and more important than party support per se.

Keywords
climate change, climate change denial, radical right, institutional distrust, ideology, political party support, sociopolitical attitudes, Green & Sustainable Science & Technology
National Category
Political Science
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-190335 (URN)10.3390/su122310226 (DOI)000597451000001 ()
Available from: 2021-02-16 Created: 2021-02-16 Last updated: 2022-02-25Bibliographically approved
Eriksson, K. & Strimling, P. (2020). Using Models to Predict Cultural Evolution From Emotional Selection Mechanisms. Emotion Review, 12(2), 79-92
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Using Models to Predict Cultural Evolution From Emotional Selection Mechanisms
2020 (English)In: Emotion Review, ISSN 1754-0739, E-ISSN 1754-0747, Vol. 12, no 2, p. 79-92Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Cultural variants may spread by being more appealing, more memorable, or less offensive than other cultural variants. Empirical studies suggest that such emotional selection is a force to be reckoned with in cultural evolution. We present a research paradigm that is suitable for the study of emotional selection. It guides empirical research by directing attention to the circumstances under which emotions influence the likelihood that an individual will influence another individual to acquire a cultural variant. We present a modeling framework to translate such knowledge into specific and testable predictions of population-level change. A set of already analyzed basic cases can serve as a toolbox.

Keywords
cultural evolution, emotional selection, imitation, social norms, social sanctions
National Category
Psychology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-178664 (URN)10.1177/1754073919890914 (DOI)000506761100001 ()
Available from: 2020-02-20 Created: 2020-02-20 Last updated: 2022-02-26Bibliographically approved
Eriksson, K., Simpson, B. & Strimling, P. (2019). Political double standards in reliance on moral foundations. Judgment and Decision Making, 14(4), 440-454
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Political double standards in reliance on moral foundations
2019 (English)In: Judgment and Decision Making, E-ISSN 1930-2975, Vol. 14, no 4, p. 440-454Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Prior research using the Moral Foundations Questionnaire (MFQ) has established that political ideology is associated with self-reported reliance on specific moral foundations in moral judgments of acts. MFQ items do not specify the agents involved in the acts, however. By specifying agents in MFQ items we revealed blatant political double standards. Conservatives thought that the same moral foundation was more relevant if victims were agents that they like (i.e., corporations and other conservatives) but less relevant when the same agents were perpetrators. Liberals showed the same pattern for agents that they like (i.e., news media and other liberals). A UK sample showed much weaker political double standards with respect to corporations and news media, consistent with feelings about corporations and news media being much less politicized in the UK than in the US. We discuss the implications for moral foundations theory.

Keywords
moral foundations, political psychology, moral judgments, rights, obligations
National Category
Psychology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-171786 (URN)10.1017/s1930297500006124 (DOI)000477796000005 ()
Available from: 2019-08-27 Created: 2019-08-27 Last updated: 2024-07-04Bibliographically approved
Jylhä, K. M., Rydgren, J. & Strimling, P. (2019). Radical right-wing voters from right and left: Comparing Sweden Democrat voters who previously voted for the Conservative Party or the Social Democratic Party. Scandinavian Political Studies, 42(3-4), 220-244
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Radical right-wing voters from right and left: Comparing Sweden Democrat voters who previously voted for the Conservative Party or the Social Democratic Party
2019 (English)In: Scandinavian Political Studies, ISSN 0080-6757, E-ISSN 1467-9477, Vol. 42, no 3-4, p. 220-244Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

As in many other European countries, the political system has undergone rapid changes in Sweden while a radical right-wing party - The Sweden Democrats (SD) - has grown from a negligible position into one of the country's largest parties. SD has been winning voters from both the right and the left sides of the political spectrum, and particularly from Sweden's two largest parties, the Conservative Party (Moderaterna, M) and the Social Democratic Party (S). The present study investigated the extent to which SD voters who previously voted for one of these two parties differ from each other, and compared these SD voters with current Conservative Party and Social Democratic voters. The results showed that 1) economic deprivation offers a better explanation for the past mobility from S, than from M, to the SD; 2) no group differences were found between previous M and S voters in attitudes connected to the appeal of an anti-establishment party; and 3) views on the profile issues espoused by the radical right, most importantly opposition to immigration, did not differ between SD voters who come from M and S. However, SD voters - particularly SD voters who had formerly voted for the Social Democratic party - differed from the voters of their previous parties in several aspects. It is thus possible that many SD voters will not return to the parties they previously voted for, at least as long as the immigration issue continues to be of high salience in the society.

National Category
Political Science
Research subject
Political Science
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-171641 (URN)10.1111/1467-9477.12147 (DOI)000477180500001 ()
Available from: 2019-08-22 Created: 2019-08-22 Last updated: 2022-02-26Bibliographically approved
Strimling, P., Vartanova, I., Jansson, F. & Eriksson, K. (2019). The connection between moral positions and moral arguments drives opinion change. Nature Human Behaviour, 3(9), 922-930
Open this publication in new window or tab >>The connection between moral positions and moral arguments drives opinion change
2019 (English)In: Nature Human Behaviour, E-ISSN 2397-3374, Vol. 3, no 9, p. 922-930Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Liberals and conservatives often take opposing positions on moral issues. But what makes a moral position liberal or conservative? Why does public opinion tend to become more liberal over time? And why does public opinion change especially fast on certain issues, such as gay rights? We offer an explanation based on how different positions connect with different kinds of moral arguments. Based on a formal model of opinion dynamics, we predicted that positions better connected to harm and fairness arguments will be more popular among liberals and will become more popular over time among liberals and conservatives. Finally, the speed of this trend will be faster the better the position connects to harm and fairness arguments. These predictions all held with high accuracy in 44years of polling on moral opinions. The model explains the connection between ideology and moral opinions, and generates precise predictions for future opinion change.

National Category
Psychology Philosophy, Ethics and Religion Other Humanities
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-174958 (URN)10.1038/s41562-019-0647-x (DOI)000485813100012 ()31308486 (PubMedID)
Available from: 2019-10-25 Created: 2019-10-25 Last updated: 2025-01-31Bibliographically approved
Strimling, P., de Barra, M. & Eriksson, K. (2018). Asymmetries in punishment propensity may drive the civilizing process. Nature Human Behaviour, 2(2), 148-155
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Asymmetries in punishment propensity may drive the civilizing process
2018 (English)In: Nature Human Behaviour, E-ISSN 2397-3374, Vol. 2, no 2, p. 148-155Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Norms about hygiene and violence have both shown a tendency to become increasingly strict, in the sense that the handling of bodily fluids and the use of violence have become increasingly restricted. The generality of this directional change across a large number of societies has not been captured by previous explanations. We propose an explanation of the directional change that is based on the aggregation of everyday interactions. This theory posits that directional norm change can come about if there is an asymmetry in punishment propensity between the people who prefer stricter norms and those who prefer looser norms. Asymmetry in punishment can arise from underlying asymmetry in the threat perceived, where a stricter-than-preferred behaviour is perceived as inherently less threatening than a looser one. We demonstrate the logic of the theory using a formal model and test some of its assumptions through survey experiments.

Keywords
History, Human behaviour, Sociology
National Category
Psychology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-156000 (URN)10.1038/s41562-017-0278-z (DOI)000429375000019 ()
Available from: 2018-05-11 Created: 2018-05-11 Last updated: 2022-02-24Bibliographically approved
Cownden, D., Eriksson, K. & Strimling, P. (2018). The implications of learning across perceptually and strategically distinct situations. Synthese, 195(2), 511-528
Open this publication in new window or tab >>The implications of learning across perceptually and strategically distinct situations
2018 (English)In: Synthese, ISSN 0039-7857, E-ISSN 1573-0964, Vol. 195, no 2, p. 511-528Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

Game theory is a formal approach to behavior that focuses on the strategic aspect of situations. The game theoretic approach originates in economics but has been embraced by scholars across disciplines, including many philosophers and biologists. This approach has an important weakness: the strategic aspect of a situation, which is its defining quality in game theory, is often not its most salient quality in human (or animal) cognition. Evidence from a wide range of experiments highlights this shortcoming. Previous theoretical and empirical work has sought to address this weakness by considering learning across an ensemble of multiple games simultaneously. Here we extend this framework, incorporating artificial neural networks, to allow for an investigation of the interaction between the perceptual and functional similarity of the games composing the larger ensemble. Using this framework, we conduct a theoretical investigation of a population that encounters both stag hunts and prisoner’s dilemmas, two situations that are strategically different but which may or may not be perceptually similar.

Keywords
Game theory, Learning, Multiple games, Bounded rationality, Framing effects, Artificial neural networks
National Category
Psychology Cultural Studies Educational Sciences
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-125205 (URN)10.1007/s11229-014-0641-9 (DOI)000422664200003 ()
Available from: 2016-01-08 Created: 2016-01-08 Last updated: 2025-02-18Bibliographically approved
Cownden, D., Eriksson, K. & Strimling, P. (2017). A popular misapplication of evolutionary modeling to the study of human cooperation. Evolution and human behavior, 38(3), 421-427
Open this publication in new window or tab >>A popular misapplication of evolutionary modeling to the study of human cooperation
2017 (English)In: Evolution and human behavior, ISSN 1090-5138, E-ISSN 1879-0607, Vol. 38, no 3, p. 421-427Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

To examine the evolutionary basis of a behavior, an established approach (known as the phenotypic gambit) is to assume that the behavior is controlled by a single allele, the fitness effects of which are derived from a consideration of how the behavior interacts, via life-history, with other ecological factors. Here we contrast successful applications of this approach with several examples of an influential and superficially similar line of research on the evolutionary basis of human cooperation. A key difference is identified: in the latter line of research the focal behavior, cooperation, is abstractly defined in terms of immediate fitness costs and benefits. Selection is then assumed to act on strategies in an iterated social context for which fitness effects can be derived by aggregation of the abstractly defined immediate fitness effects over a lifetime. This approach creates a closed theoretical loop, rendering models incapable of making predictions or providing insight into the origin of human cooperation. We conclude with a discussion of how evolutionary approaches might be appropriately used in the study of human social behavior.

Keywords
Altruism, Cultural evolution, Evolutionary game theory, Genetic evolution, Human cooperation, Phenotypic gambit
National Category
Cultural Studies
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-143397 (URN)10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2017.01.004 (DOI)000399966800018 ()
Available from: 2017-05-31 Created: 2017-05-31 Last updated: 2022-02-28Bibliographically approved
Eriksson, K., Strimling, P., Andersson, P. A. & Lindholm, T. (2017). Costly punishment in the ultimatum game evokes moral concern, in particular when framed as payoff reduction. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 69, 59-64
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Costly punishment in the ultimatum game evokes moral concern, in particular when framed as payoff reduction
2017 (English)In: Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, ISSN 0022-1031, E-ISSN 1096-0465, Vol. 69, p. 59-64Article in journal (Refereed) Published
Abstract [en]

The ultimatum game is a common economic experiment in which some participants reject another's unfair offer of how to split some money, even though it leaves them both worse off. This costly behavior can be seen as enforcement of a fairness norm and has been labeled “altruistic punishment”, suggesting that it is a moral thing to do. But is this behavior viewed as moral by participants? Is it viewed as punishment? And are the payoff consequences of the behavior sufficient to determine the answers to these questions? To investigate this we framed costly punishment in two different ways: either as rejection of an offer (the standard ultimatum game framing) or as reduction of payoff. In a series of paid and hypothetical experiments we found that moral concerns about costly punishment depended on the framing. Specifically, the reduction frame elicited more moral concern about, and less use of, costly punishment than did the rejection frame. Several implications are discussed.

Keywords
costly punishment, ultimatum game, norm enforcement, motives, framing, moral judgment
National Category
Cultural Studies Psychology
Research subject
Psychology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-137890 (URN)10.1016/j.jesp.2016.09.004 (DOI)000392774500006 ()
Available from: 2017-01-13 Created: 2017-01-13 Last updated: 2022-02-28Bibliographically approved
Organisations
Identifiers
ORCID iD: ORCID iD iconorcid.org/0000-0002-9750-5835

Search in DiVA

Show all publications