Change search
CiteExportLink to record
Permanent link

Direct link
Cite
Citation style
  • apa
  • ieee
  • modern-language-association-8th-edition
  • vancouver
  • Other style
More styles
Language
  • de-DE
  • en-GB
  • en-US
  • fi-FI
  • nn-NO
  • nn-NB
  • sv-SE
  • Other locale
More languages
Output format
  • html
  • text
  • asciidoc
  • rtf
Do you recognize hon, han and hen?: How binary and gender-neutral pronouns affect gender categorization in face recognition
Stockholm University, Faculty of Social Sciences, Department of Psychology.ORCID iD: 0000-0002-3929-6019
Stockholm University, Faculty of Social Sciences, Department of Psychology.ORCID iD: 0000-0001-5398-2894
Lund University, Sweden.
Kristianstad University, Sweden.ORCID iD: 0000-0001-6593-2464
Show others and affiliations
(English)Manuscript (preprint) (Other academic)
Abstract [en]

This study tested how pronouns affect gender categorization in a face recognition task. Participants (N = 362) memorized a gender-ambiguous face presented with either a binary pronoun (she, he) a gender-neutral pronoun (hen) or without a pronoun (control) into memory. In the recognition task, participants searched for the gender-ambiguous face in a series of typically feminine and masculine faces. Results indicated that participants who memorized the face with a feminine pronoun spent more time looking at feminine faces than masculine faces when searching for the target face. The opposite was found when the face was presented with a masculine pronoun. This shows that binary pronouns activate binary gender categorization, which speeds up the process of face recognition. Presenting a gender-ambiguous face with a gender-neutral pronoun led to participants spending more time looking at feminine faces than masculine faces, which may be due to the stimuli being biased towards femininity. However, participants who reported not having categorized the target face spent an equal time looking at feminine and masculine faces, which may indicate that the gender-ambiguous face had not been gender categorized.

Keywords [en]
gender-fair language, pronouns, hen, gender categorization
National Category
Psychology
Research subject
Psychology
Identifiers
URN: urn:nbn:se:su:diva-195792OAI: oai:DiVA.org:su-195792DiVA, id: diva2:1587893
Note

Unpublished manuscript part of dissertation.

Available from: 2021-08-25 Created: 2021-08-25 Last updated: 2022-02-25Bibliographically approved
In thesis
1. Breaking the Binary: Attitudes towards and cognitive effects of gender-neutral pronouns
Open this publication in new window or tab >>Breaking the Binary: Attitudes towards and cognitive effects of gender-neutral pronouns
2021 (English)Doctoral thesis, comprehensive summary (Other academic)
Abstract [en]

For a long time, Swedish only had two third-person singular pronouns: hon [‘she’] and han [‘he’]. Following several publications using the gender-neutral pronoun hen to refer to its characters, a debate article in a national newspaper proposed expanding the Swedish pronouns with hen. This proposal ignited a nation-wide debate on the use of a gender-neutral pronoun and its potential consequences. Proponents of hen believed that hen would make the language more gender-fair by making gender less salient, and by having a pronoun for nonbinary gender individuals. Opponents believed hen would confuse children and be trivial for achieving gender equality. This response shows that hen challenges beliefs on what language should look like, how gender is defined, and how gender should be represented in language. In this thesis, I have documented beliefs about hen. I have mapped the initial resistance, and found underlying motivations for different types of criticism. In two experimental studies, I tested whether common arguments in the debate are supported by empirical evidence, and whether hen can affect the way others are perceived.

Study I documented the content of the criticism of hen. As a background and coding scheme I used research on criticism of past gender-fair language initiatives, such as the replacing of generic ‘he’ with ‘he or she’. I found that the criticism of hen was largely the same as in the 1970s and 1980s. Subsequently, I generated four dimensions of underlying motivations that characterize criticism of gender-fair language. These dimensions of criticisms can be considered and addressed in different ways when implementing and researching gender-fair language.

Study II tested whether hen indeed is more distracting in written communication than hon or han. Participants read sentences in which hen referred to role nouns that varied in how strongly they were associated with a gender. The results indicated that hen had a small processing cost compared to hon or han, and there was no difference between lexically gendered or stereotypically gendered role nouns. The common argument that hen is a strong distractor in written communication was thus not supported by these findings. 

Study III examined how pronouns influence gender categorization and the memory recall of a face. The results indicated that participants were more likely to categorize a gender-ambiguous face as a woman when presented with a feminine pronoun, and that they spent more time looking at feminine faces than masculine faces in the memory task. The opposite was found when the gender-ambiguous face was presented with a masculine pronoun. Encoding a gender-ambiguous face with hen partially eliminated the gender categorization effect. The results show that binary pronouns activate binary gender categorization and that gender-neutral pronouns can reduce such categorization. 

The findings in this thesis provide insights into the early stages of the implementation of a gender-neutral pronoun, and its potential to affect social cognition. It shows that criticism of hen is fueled by a set of ideological convictions and practical concerns, which it shares with criticism of other gender-fair language initiatives. In addition, hen leads to criticism of breaking the woman-man gender binary. This thesis provides early evidence for the potential of gender-neutral language, such as hen, to reduce biases in social cognition.

Place, publisher, year, edition, pages
Stockholm: Department of Psychology, Stockholm University, 2021. p. 99
Keywords
gender-fair language, gender-neutral pronouns, hen
National Category
Psychology
Research subject
Psychology
Identifiers
urn:nbn:se:su:diva-195457 (URN)978-91-7911-584-5 (ISBN)978-91-7911-585-2 (ISBN)
Public defence
2021-10-08, Hörsal 5, Albanovägen 14 and online via Zoom, public link is available at the department website, Stockholm, 10:00 (English)
Opponent
Supervisors
Available from: 2021-09-15 Created: 2021-08-18 Last updated: 2022-02-25Bibliographically approved

Open Access in DiVA

No full text in DiVA

Authority records

Vergoossen, Hellen P.Gustafsson Sendén, MarieRenström, Emma A.Sczesny, Sabine

Search in DiVA

By author/editor
Vergoossen, Hellen P.Gustafsson Sendén, MarieRenström, Emma A.Sczesny, Sabine
By organisation
Department of Psychology
Psychology

Search outside of DiVA

GoogleGoogle Scholar

urn-nbn

Altmetric score

urn-nbn
Total: 565 hits
CiteExportLink to record
Permanent link

Direct link
Cite
Citation style
  • apa
  • ieee
  • modern-language-association-8th-edition
  • vancouver
  • Other style
More styles
Language
  • de-DE
  • en-GB
  • en-US
  • fi-FI
  • nn-NO
  • nn-NB
  • sv-SE
  • Other locale
More languages
Output format
  • html
  • text
  • asciidoc
  • rtf